New to word finding therapy?

I recently began therapy with a new word finding client.  His lack of understanding about what “word finding” or “word retrieval” is was a good reminder to go back to the basics and explain why he is in language therapy.  This led me back to my very first blog post in 2014.  I have edited it and reprinted below:

So where do I start when I have a new speech therapy client and have identified word finding as a goal area? So often, we jump straight into expressive language activities. I feel very strongly, however, that we first need to get our young friend on board: if he is to be successful in therapy, he needs to be aware of why he is here and why it will benefit him.

When we begin articulation therapy, we first make sure a child can discriminate the “old” sound from the “new” sound, right? Shouldn’t we also be sure a child can recognize what a “word finding problem” is? Shouldn’t he be able to “discriminate” when he is stuck on a target word and when he is using the correct vocabulary word?

Step # 1 is AWARENESS. A child in language therapy needs to be aware that there is such a thing as a “word finding problem.” EVERYONE has word finding difficulties at some point in time….Mom, Dad , teachers, coaches, smart kids, cool kids. But if these difficulties occur too often, they can really interfere with our trying to express ourselves.

No child wants to be confronted with his difficulties when he first meets the new SLP. We have to strike the delicate balance between helping him become aware of his challenges and making him feel like a failure. So I start by helping him listen for the word finding difficulties others have.

Look at the media!   TV and radio commercials are full of examples. His “speech homework” can be to find an example of a character in a favorite show stymied by getting stuck on the word he needs. At home, if he can catch his parent calling him a sibling’s name, he realizes that we are all “word finders.” Likewise, what about the teacher who says, “After math, we will go to music…I mean art”?

I have created a number of dialogs that I use to help my WF students become more aware of the types of word finding problems they may experience. You can write them yourself. The more specific they are to an individual child, the more meaningful they are. But I have provided a few for you to get started on my “resources” page.  Have the kids read it themselves, or read it to them and have them push a button to identify a WF “mistake.”

What ideas have you used to create AWARENESS of word finding challenges? Please share!

New research about MEMORY

Remember those neuroscience courses in which we learned that we form short-term memories first, and later they convert to long-term memory?  Perhaps we were wrong!  New research published in Science last week suggests that our brain simultaneously makes TWO copies of our memories: one in the hippocampus and one in the cortex. Using a new research tool called optogenetics, researchers in Japan implanted a tiny fiber-optic cable in genetically engineered mice, which allowed them to turn specific neurons on and off.  Therefore, they could better understand the function of those neurons.

Both the cortex and the hippocampus lit up simultaneously when new memories were recorded. The scientists found that for the first few days, the neurons in the hippocampus were the only ones that fired during retrieval of memories.  The neurons in the cortex matured later, and at that point they lit up when a memory was recalled.

I’m always intrigued by neuroscience.  This new research may help us understand how retrieval works in the brain.  The more we understand, the more specific and efficient we can be in our language therapy.

Here’s the PBS article that cited the Science study: http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/nova/next/body/our-brains-instantly-make-two-copies-of-each-memory/

Filler words in conversation?

A new article in Business Insider  cites a study about the psychological reasons we use filler words, such as “um, uh, er.”   It was a good reminder that we ALL use filler words, not just individuals with word finding difficulties.   Dr. Michael Handford, professor of applied linguistics and English language at Cardiff University in the UK, explained that people often use fillers to be polite.  Linguists have suggested that speakers who use more fillers are probably being more conscious of who they are talking to and what they are saying.

“As speakers we are often aware [that] if we speak too complexly the listener might not understand,” Handford told The Independent. “We use these items, pretty unconsciously, to help the person process what we are saying.”  Fillers are also used to buy time when we know what we want to say, but our brain hasn’t yet caught up.

 

Fillers can also signal deception. A retired FBI Special Agent and professor at Western Illinois University, Dr. John R. Schafer, was cited in a blog post in Psychology Today :   https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/let-their-words-do-the-talking/201512/um-little-words-can-signal-big-lies-you-know  He said tag words such as “you know,” “I mean,” and “right” may be used to seek confirmation in the listener, or convince them of the truthfulness of  a message.

So does that mean using fillers means you are lying?  By no means.  Filler words can simply be a habit.  So how do we as SLPs interpret those interjections and delays? 

Do the fillers interfere with conversation?  Are they typical for this individual?  Consider the context; consider the speaker.  We all experience word finding difficulties at times.  We have a more difficult time retrieving the words we need when stressed or fatigued.  We may resort to fillers to let ourselves (or our listeners) catch up.  The article reminded me that we shouldn’t look at one language sample on one day and diagnose word finding problems.  We need to assess if there is a pattern.  We need to assess if communication is really impaired.

The Business Insider article may be found at:  http://www.businessinsider.com/psychological-reasons-we-use-filler-words-2017-4?utm_source=feedly&utm_medium=webfeeds

Let me know what you think!

 

Javelina Ballerina

I just returned from short trip to Arizona.  There’s nothing like seeing a different part of the country to remind you that retrieval is all about frequency of use and recency of use.  For the last  four months, I have been using words like snow, sleet, wind-chill, and blizzard.  Suddenly I was using words like Sonoran, saguaro, and …oh, my, what are those ugly pig-looking animals called??  Javelinas!  (Actually, I think the plural is javelina)  It was a good reminder that when vocabulary words haven’t been used in a while, they are difficult to retrieve.  I would not expect my midwest kids to be rapid retrievers of the same words that kids in the southwest use regularly.  We need to choose words that are functional to help our word finders!

Please remember Dr. Diane German’s recommendations for mnemonic cues.  A detailed description is found here: https://www.wordfindingforkids.com/an-endorsement-from-the-word-finding-guru/

As a quick review:

  1. Select a word that is difficult for the  learner to retrieve.
  2. Divide the target word into syllables, and reinforce each syllable of multi-syllable words.
  3. Associate similar-sounding, easy-to-retrieve words with “evasive” syllables.
  4. Say the target word aloud 5 times.
  5. Make up a meaningful sentence using the word.

Please remember to think of the cue as you practice saying the target word out loud.

So how did I remember javelina?  I pictured a javelina wearing a pink tutu — javelina ballerina!  Ridiculous?  Yes, but certainly an image I am not likely to forget!

 

 

So much to talk about!

Sweet 16, Elite 8, Final 4.  But wait!  There is spring break to talk about.  And what about the craziness going on in Washington?Health care, tax reform, Supreme Court hearings… There are also new curriculum words to practice.  Bottom line, there are many topics on the minds of our language therapy clients.  It is important to individualize and anticipate the vocabulary each will need to retrieve.  This is a great time to create a homework assignment!  Ask each of your speech kids to come up with 3 words they may need to retrieve quickly in the week ahead.  What better way to promote awareness and self-advocacy than to have them think of their own cues.

Encourage them to come up with their own mnemonic cues (hint: here is the process – https://www.wordfindingforkids.com/an-endorsement-from-the-word-finding-guru/).

I also wrote a dialog for kids to practice some of the words they will need for a spring break journey into Chicago. https://www.wordfindingforkids.com/spring-break-word-finding-dialog/.   Maybe it will spark some ideas to come up with a dialog of your own!  If so, please share!  I would love to publish some of your own mnemonic cues and therapy ideas.

More on Math and Language

Last week, I talked about the worthwhile workshop I attended on the topic of math and language.  There is so much more to share!  Thank you again to Karen Tzanetopoulos, M.S., CCC-SLP, for sharing her expertise.  Here is the link to last week’s post: https://www.wordfindingforkids.com/math-and-word-finding-an-unlikely-combination/

More on the important language basis of math

Think of how many ways we say “approximate” – about, around, approximately, almost, roughly, close to, nearly, barely, maybe, just about, just over, more or less, not more than, -ish.  And how does a student know when it is appropriate to choose one term instead of another?  Yet learning to approximate is an important communication skill.  Not only does it help us make predictions and solve problems more quickly, but it also helps us know if our answers make sense.

Our American measurement system is not the logical, predictable metric system used by most of the world.  Inch, foot, yard, and mile are totally arbitrary. How about ounce, pound, and ton?  Children in the U.S. have a huge disadvantage in learning to measure when compared to their peers in other countries.

Words used in a math context have very different meanings than words used outside of math.  Negative numbers, fractions, remainders, proper numbers, mixed numbers, point, and reduce are just a few of the terms that mean something very different outside of the math room.  And even within math class – does “eighth” mean eighth in line, or does it mean one-eighth of a whole?

Karen’s solution is to use concrete manipulatives and a physical number line.  She has a great list of resources and materials, which you can ask her about at karentzan@gmail.com.   Sadly, the toys with which kids learn these concepts are disappearing from preschool and early elementary classrooms.  Remember when young kids could play in the grocery store and “buy” a dozen eggs or piece together a wooden pie or a pizza divided into sixths or eighths?  Our attempts to push kids ahead in math have resulted in many children who can’t conceptualize the meaning of numbers and formulas that are written on a page.

Fractions and decimals bring their own set of challenges.  What does “point” mean?  And why do the numbers to the right of the decimal point have different values and different names than the numbers to the left?  Instead of multiplying fractions, kids first need to experiment with concrete objects and understand what the numbers represent.

We compound the difficulty when we get to word problems.  The syntax of word problems can be very confusing! Here’s an example from Karen’s presentation:

Jose took the 26 baseball cards he no longer wanted and gave them to Brian.

Now Jose has 71 baseball cards left.  How many baseball cards did Jose have

to begin with?

If we use the common strategy of encouraging a student to look for the key words “how many are left?,” the child will immediately assume it is a subtraction problem.  But in this case, he needs to add!  And what about that imbedded clause, “he no longer wanted”?  A child with syntax difficulties is likely to miss a problem like this because of his language deficit, not because he can’t do the math. If he is further asked to “explain his answer,” he will probably put his head on his desk in frustration and give up.

I googled “first grade word problems” and came up with the following example common core problem:

Jen has 7 apples. Pat has 10 apples. How many fewer apples does Jen have than Pat?  

First grade!  So a child is just learning to read, and he has to figure out “how many fewer than…?”  But if I give him a basket of apples, one with 7 apples and one with 10 apples, he can play with them, feel them, and count them.  He is much more likely to figure out that lucky Pat has 3 more apples than Jen.

I have proposed more problems than solutions with my synopsis of Karen’s presentation.  She brought blocks, physical number lines, and other manipulatives to demonstrate many ways to support our language-disordered students as they tackle the complex language of math.  She proposed schema that help students organize and understand their information. By providing accommodations and scaffolding complex information, we can help them be successful in all levels of their math education.

Math can be a real challenge! Let’s do our part to parse out the language that is compounding our kids’ difficulties.

 

 

 

Math and word finding – an unlikely combination?

I almost skipped a continuing education opportunity on Saturday morning.  Between the chance to sleep in on a Saturday and the time change to daylight savings time overnight into Sunday, I relished the extra couple of hours of sleep I might grab.  I’m so very glad I attended my North Suburban Speech and Language Association workshop instead!  I spent a very worthwhile three hours learning about the important role we speech-language pathologists can play supporting our students in math.  Math?  Yes, math!

I have used word finding cues occasionally with some of the multi-syllable words like isosceles, perpendicular, and circumference.  But I didn’t pay much attention to the huge language foundation that underlies mathematics, and the scaffolding we can provide to ensure that our language-impaired students aren’t penalized because of their disability.

Karen Tzanetopoulos, M.S., CCC-SLP, presented on the topic, “The Abstract Language of Math and How the SLP Can Help.”  Karen is an SLP in the Chicago area who is in private practice.   In the course of her research, she met with over 130 teachers, math specialists, and administrators around the country and found a crying need  for help in teaching math to children with dyslexia and language learning differences. Her passion for this topic led her to pursue (and receive!) National Science Foundation grants.

One of Karen’s primary points was that although math exists without language, in order to communicate about math, we must use language.  English, in particular, is full of homophones and complex, abstract terms that can be particularly confusing: One, won, to, too, two, for, four, fore are just a start.  Even the names we have for numbers are phonologically complex (three, six, seven). Whereas children in China learn numbers whose sound has a correlation with its meaning, we have words like “eleven” and “thirteen” which don’t correspond to base 10.  Think of how difficult it is for a child with auditory discrimination or processing difficulties to understand that “fourth” is different from “four”, and “four” has nothing to do with “for”  or “fore.”

Karen stressed the value of developing a mental number line so one can develop a number sense.   In her words, every child should have “extensive experience with physical number lines and practice counting and performing calculations on real number lines.” She starts with concrete, physical number lines, then moves on to paper.  Only later can a child develop a mental number line.  Without number sense, kids have no clue when their answers make no sense.  So what does this have to do with word finding?

Just like rapid, automatic naming is a strong indicator of reading success, early accuracy and speed of number naming and counting fluency are strongly correlated with math success.  We need to work on counting with preschoolers. Count, count, count:  count on fingers, slow it down, make it meaningful.  Make it concrete and visual.  Counting needs to become automatic.  Then we can move on to those tricky position and quantity words.  Think about how confusing “follows” is.  If you follow someone, you are behind that person, right?  But in math, if I ask for “the number that follows 7″ I am asking for the one that comes after it, but “ahead” of it in the left-to-right demands of reading.  The language of math is very complex!   And take a look at these numbers:

                      3

8

Which number is larger?

Then we get to math facts, which have a heavy phonological burden and require quick word retrieval.  Karen stressed remembering a hierarchy from concrete to representational to abstract teaching.  Start with concrete  teaching of numbers and number facts.  Use manipulatives; let kids experiment and discover how numbers are related.  Help them find the patterns.  Remember that we use many words to say the same thing.   For example, in addition, we might say plus, add, all together, altogether, total, both, sum, and in all, increased by, combined, added to, or additional.  In subtraction, we might say minus, subtract, take away, less than, remain, left, lost, reduce, how much more, how much less, or nearer.  Yikes! No wonder kids get confused.

We haven’t even begun to talk about multiplication, division, fractions, and decimals. This is only a start.  Karen had so much great information to share!  Hopefully I have given you food for thought (oooh, boy, another idiom)  and a starting point to think about the important role you play in supporting math for kids with language difficulties.

Please send me your comments or questions.  You can also reach out to Karen directly via her email:  karentzan@gmail.com.  She would be happy to hear from you!   I hope to follow up with some of your questions and her responses in future posts.

 

 

 

 

 

Students who are not aware of their word finding difficulties…

A reader wrote the following question, and Dr. German was kind enough to respond.  I asked if I could share her comments with all of you…

Thank you for your post. I work in a private elementary school and I use your TOWF-2 to evaluate many of my students. I also have your book “It’s on the Tip of My Tongue. My biggest challenge with my students who have WFD is that most of them are not very aware of their difficulties. Therefore when I ask them what words they often get stuck on they don’t know of any. Most of my students don’t have specific words that are consistently hard for them. It’s different words every time. Therefore I can’t use many of the strategies you recommend. What do you recommend for students who don’t have consistent words that they get stuck on?

Dr. German writes:

Thank you so much for your question. I am pleased you use the Test of Word Finding, Second Edition (TWF-2).

For Word Finding (WF) intervention, I recommend that you select known vocabulary from the following 4 areas:

  • words or names that the student self reports he/she is having difficulty retrieving;
  • words or names parents or teachers report the learner is having difficulty retrieving;

3) names of classmates and teachers; and

4) academic words (common core vocabulary) that you as the SLP predict the student may have difficulty retrieving.

It appears you have been focusing on # 1 above. Your learner may not be able to yet reflect on his/her own language so as to be able to tell you when he/she experiences a WF block or makes a WF substitution. Thus I would focus on areas 2 -4 below. As you work on your students’ WF, they will become more aware of their Word Finding Difficulties (WFD) and the words that they find evasive, the ones that they cannot think of.

SLPs can predict what words in the curriculum that might be difficult for a learner to retrieve. It is these words that SLPs need to treat during the learner’s language lesson. Findings from research can guide SLPs. For example, research (German & Newman, 2004) has shown that certain lexical factors of words (length, phonological complexity, neighborhood density, and frequency of occurrence,) can make words easier or more difficult to retrieve than others. For example, it has been reported that learners with WFD often have difficulty retrieving:

  • the multisyllabic words in their curriculum (longer words can be more difficult to retrieve, like Mississippi in language arts);
  • the words that have low phonotactic probability (words with rare phonological sequences can be more difficult to retrieve, hypothesis in science);
  • the words that have small word families (words that reside in sparse neighborhoods can be more difficult to retrieve, like Xerox in the office); and
  • the rarer words (words with lower frequency of occurrence can be more difficulty to retrieve, like sphere in math).

Using this research as a guideline, SLP’s can identify those curriculum words that might be most challenging to a learner with WFD and apply retrieval strategies so as to head off WFD in the classroom.

I hope my response was helpful. Best, Diane G.

Reference

German, D. J., & Newman, R. S. (2004). The impact of lexical factors on children’s word finding errors. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(3), 624-636.

 

Mystery words

One of my readers asked about helping her students with words that they have not anticipated.  The research tells us that retrieval improves for the words we work on.  However, there is not generalization to other words.  How frustrating!  So how do we help with those other words?

This is why I think kids need STRATEGIES.  They need to know how to help themselves when they are stuck. Different strategies work for different kids.  I try to present to them a toolkit, and encourage them to use the tool that fits their own needs.   This is also a teaching moment.  What is a strategy?  I remind them that their favorite sports heroes use strategies all the time.  It’s a game plan, a way to win a game, a plan of action.

Visualizing is very powerful for many.  Some kids need to be taught to picture things in their minds.  Another strategy is to try a closure task (“The tool I use to turn a screw is a ….”, or “At gymnastics class, I walk on the narrow….” ).  Some students help themselves by silently going through the alphabet and thinking of beginning sounds (NOT letters, but sounds).  This may jump start the phonological connection to their target word.  Is she a child who uses a lot of gestures?  Great – encourage that.  Anything that promotes communication is a positive step.  Think of associated words.   Can’t think of the name of those things you wear on your legs during a soccer game?  Think of soccer, goal, forward, guard, jersey,….Oh, that’s it!  Shinguards!  Automatic associations can be helpful, and there are many resources that have lists and lists of associations (peanut butter and….,  bread and…., hammer and….).  Learning categories is another great way to promote connections among words.

Once he “finds” the word, have him repeat, repeat, repeat. Repeat the word aloud. Use it in a meaningful sentence. It’s empowering to know he can help himself in the moment.

Please remember that retrieval is affected by frequency of use and recency of use.  Help a child plan ahead:  what words will he need?  Practice those words aloud!  But there will always be words that trip him up unexpectedly.  So have a game plan – a strategy – for those moments.

 

Dr. Diane German writes a guest post – “Ask Yourself, Are You Doing Enough for Your Learners with Word Finding Difficulties?

I am delighted to welcome Dr. Diane German, author of the Test of Word Finding – Third Edition (TWF-3) and the Word Finding Intervention Program – Second Edition (WFIP -2) . Both are available from PRO.ED (see below).  Dr. German has also been my mentor in the area of word finding.   

 Ask Yourself, Are You Doing Enough For Your Learners With Word Finding Difficulties (WFD)?

Often I hear Speech and Language Pathologists (SLPs) tell me that they are concerned that they are not doing enough to facilitate the students in their schools and centers who display WFD. They ask me how and where can they improve services for these students. Because SLPs are the main advocates for students with WFD, I am happy to address this question in hopes that it will be helpful to SLPs and will improve services for students with WFD. Below, I address 3 programing areas where professionals may not be doing enough for students with WFD: WF Identification, WF assessment, and WF intervention.

It is not enough to just report that a learner has a WFD. Simply stating that a learner has a WFD may be insufficiently detailed to plan WF intervention. Research has indicated that students manifest WFD not only when retrieving single words or names, but in discourse contexts also; and that their WFD can be suggestive of three distinct error patterns. For example, it has been reported that students with WFD may manifest difficulty in retrieving the semantic aspects of the target word (Error Pattern 1, Lemma Related Semantic errors, eel for octopus, commonly know as a “slip of the tongue”,); the form properties of the word (Error Pattern 2, Form Related Blocked Error, …IDK or I Pass, commonly known as a “tip of the tongue error”); or the complete phonological schema of the target word, (Error Pattern 3, Form and Segment Related Phonologic Error noculars for binoculars, commonly known as a “twist of the tongue”).   If so, just concluding a learner has a WFD may overlook important distinctions between error patterns that could provide useful information as to the source of a child’s WFD, and likely the best approaches for his/her WF intervention.

It is not enough to assess WFD with picture naming only. WF assessment needs to be more than just naming pictures, if meaningful, strategic, WF intervention is going to be planned. Rather a deep assessment in WF needs to occur if a students WF error patterns are gong to be identified and addressed. The goal of WF assessment is the differential diagnosis of WF error patterns.

The Test of Word Finding, Third Edition (TWF-3) (German, 2015, full disclosure I am the author of the TWF-3) is a standardized measure designed to complete the needed differential diagnosis of WF in single word naming contexts. The TWF-3 goes beyond picture naming providing six informal diagnostic procedures to determine learners’ individual WF error patterns. These procedures and corresponding Error Patterns include:

(1) Contrasts between word knowledge (comprehension) and word production (lexical access) on the same words to identify performance discrepancies between knowing and retrieving a word (needed to diagnosis WFD).

(2) Phonemic cueing procedures to assess blocked pathways between semantic and phonological representations (identifies Error Pattern 2, commonly known as a “tip of the tongue”);

(3) Imitation procedure to rule out articulation difficulties (identifies Error Pattern 3, commonly known as a twist of the tongue);

(4) Response analysis to observe if access to semantic and/or form features are derailed or blocked during the WF process (Semantic substitutions can identify Error Pattern 1; no responses identify Error Pattern 2; and phonemic substitutions identify Error Pattern 3);

(5) Response time analysis to observe WF efficiency (fast/inaccurate profile identifies Error Pattern 1; slow/inaccurate profile identifies Error Pattern 2 or 3; slow/ accurate profile identifies Error Pattern 2).

(6) Secondary characteristics tally (gestures or extra verbalizations during the WF disruption) to determine the learner’s metacognitive knowledge of the WF process, (“I know it, but can’t think of it.”) and metalinguistic awareness (“It starts with the P sound.”) of the evasive target word (identifies Error Pattern 2).

In summary, an in-depth WF assessment is needed to carry out a differential diagnosis in WF so as to maximize the efficacy of intervention. Together data from all these informal assessments enables examiners to predict the nature of learners’ WF disruptions. Use of vocabulary tests that simply assess picture-naming accuracy are not comprehensive enough to identify the three WF error patterns.

 

It is not enough to teach only word meanings to learners with WFD. Although current models of vocabulary instruction result in most learners being able to access known words, their focus on learning semantic representations of words is not enough for learners with WFD. This is because students with WFD are not successful in bridging their newly learned vocabulary and their lexical access skills. They thus have difficulty retrieving learned words in school assessments, classroom discourse, and in conversations (German, 2005).

Learners with WFD need a differentiated approach to vocabulary instruction. After learning word meanings (Montgomery, 2007), these students need retrieval strategies to anchor target words for automatic usage. For these words, instruction needs to focus on elaboration of words’ retrieval strength, thereby increasing the ease with which these known words can be accessed. The Word Finding Intervention Program, Second Edition (WFIP-2) (German, 2005, For full disclosure I am the author of the WFIP-2) provides retrieval strategies for words students know. For students who demonstrate Error Pattern 1 (commonly known as a “slip of the tongue” meta cognitive strategies are recommended to reduce semantic-based WF disruptions. These include Strategic Pausing, Self Monitoring (Hanson, 1996), and Self Correction (Paul, 2001) designed to reduce fast inaccurate responses. Strategic pausing helps the learner slow down the speaking process by inserting a pause before the noun in the noun phrase and/ or before the verb or adverb in the verb phrase. When strategically placed, the pause provides the speaker time to inhibit competing names or words and select the target. Self-Monitoring and Self-Correcting are taught with strategic pausing to reduce misspeaking and aid self-corrections

A three-pronged strategic approach is recommended to stabilize retrieval of words whose form learners cannot access consistently (Error Pattern 2, commonly known as the “tip of the tongue”) or retrieve partially (Error Pattern 3, commonly known as the “twist of the tongue”). This approach strives to make evasive words salient in the learner’s lexicon, anchoring retrieval of these words for future usage. It begins with the metalinguistic reinforcement of target vocabulary, e.g., segmentation to reinforce awareness of the word’s syllabic structure experiment (ex per i ment). This metalinguistic strategy is used in tandem with the second strategy, the phonological mnemonic strategy.

Mnemonic retrieval strategies target lexical access between semantic and form based processes. Learners associate phonological mnemonics to their evasive targets to anchor their retrieval for future usage. This involves linking words (e.g., X and spear for experiment) or phrases (cumulate junk for cumulus) similar in sound form to the target word or word parts. The third strategy is rehearsal. Learners are taught to think of their phonological mnemonic cue (X and Spear), while rehearing aloud their target alone (experiment) and in a sentence (We did an experiment in science.) until they are automatic in its usage.

In conclusion, the phonological mnemonic protocol makes the word’s form more salient by providing (1) metalinguistic reinforcement of the word’s syllabic structure; (2) a phonological prompt as a mnemonic link to the word form; and (3) rehearsal. It differs from other intervention protocols as its focus is on anchoring word forms to facilitate future retrieval rather than retrieval on demand, the purpose of cueing with the first sound of the target word.  

So this New Year, ask yourself are you doing enough for your students with WFD? If not, empower yourself to do more on behalf of these students. You are the only one who has the needed background in child language to do so.

References

German, D. (2014). Test of Word Finding, Third Edition (TWF-3). PRO.ED. Austin, TX.

German, D. J. (2005). Word Finding Intervention Program, Second Edition (WFIP-2). PRO.ED. Austin, TX.

Hanson, M. (1996). Self-management through self-monitoring. In Jones, K. & Charlton, T. (Eds.), Overcoming learning and behaviour difficulties: Partnership with pupils, Routledge, London, pp 173-191.

Montgomery, J. K. (2007). Evidence based strategies for vocabulary instruction/intervention, In Denti, L. & Guerin, G. (Eds.), Effective practice for adolescents with reading and literacy challenges, Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group, New York, NY, pp. 25-43.

Paul, R. (2001). Language Disorders form Infancy through Adolescence, Second Edition. Mosby, Philadelphia, PA.

Thank you, Dr. German!  Boldface emphases are my own.